Cookies on this website
We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you click 'Continue' we will assume that you are happy to receive all cookies and you will not see this message again. Click 'Find out more' for information on how to change your cookie settings.

A new study by the Lak group reveals a novel facet of dopamine signalling during visual decision making.

Dopamine neurons innervate the mouse brain when the mouse sees stimuli on a screen © Dr Armin Lak
Left: A mouse brain section: highlighted area (green) shows axons of dopamine neurons innervating striatum. Top right: Mouse eye captured while it sees stimuli on the screen. Bottom right: Structure of a dopamine molecule.

The dopamine system is the brain’s central hub for learning. Dopamine neurons are active when we receive a reward, and also when we perceive a cue predicting that we are about to receive a reward. These signals are known to be critical for reward learning. Dopamine neurons are located deep in the midbrain, and send dopamine signals to different regions of brain, most prominently to the striatum, a large brain region located under the cortex. The dopamine release within the striatum is believed to be responsible for our ability to learn, for instance, making associations between an action and a reward. However, a central question for scientists to answer is how does dopamine help us make decisions based on visual information, when we need to evaluate visual information and act accordingly? We make many visual decisions every day, and often very quickly. For example, when driving at junctions, we need to rapidly decide whether the car is approaching from the left or the right side. Another example is playing tennis, where we rapidly decide whether the incoming ball will land inside the line. Does dopamine play a role in such visually driven decisions? Can dopamine help optimise the actions that we take in response to visual stimuli? Could different groups of dopamine neurons play different roles in this process?

To approach these questions, the Lak Group took advantage of genetic, imaging and behavioural tools in mice. Mice were trained to perform a simple visual decision-making task previously developed by the group and their collaborators. Mice learned to turn a wheel to the left or right in response to visual stimuli appearing on the left or right side of a screen, receiving a small drop of juice when correct. Researchers found that imaging the activity of dopamine terminals in the dorsal striatum during this task lead to new observations that the incoming signals did not respond to visual stimuli on both sides of the body. According to Dr Armin Lak: “Dopamine signals arriving to the dorsal part of striatum signal something special: they rapidly responded to visual stimuli and actions, but they did so only for stimuli appearing on one side of the body, the contralateral side, to be precise. This is particularly interesting because it has been long known that the brain’s visual cortex is also only responding to contralateral stimuli. Thus, the lateralised dopamine signals are perhaps the precise type of signals that striatum needs in order to make simple associations between visual stimuli and actions, for example for teaching the mouse to turn its body towards food when seeing a piece of food.” Further experiments showed that this lateralised signalling is relatively exclusive to dopamine in the dorsal striatum: measuring dopamine in other regions of striatum, such as the ventral striatum, showed far fewer of these lateralised signals. Dr Lak said: “These results are beginning to uncover the roles dopamine across the brain plays in shaping and optimising our decision-making process.”

The full paper “Dopamine axons in dorsal striatum encode contralateral visual stimuli and choices” is available to read in The Journal of Neuroscience.

Similar stories

Drug could help diabetic hearts recover after a heart attack

New research led by Associate Professor Lisa Heather has found that a drug known as molidustat, currently in clinical trials for another condition, could reduce risk of heart failure after heart attacks.

Blood bank storage can reduce ability of transfusions to treat anaemia

New research from the Swietach Group in collaboration with NHS Blood and Transplant has demonstrated that the process of storing blood in blood banks can negatively impact the function of red blood cells and consequently may reduce the effectiveness of blood transfusions, a treatment commonly used to combat anaemia.

Overlapping second messengers increase dynamic control of physiological responses

New research from the Parekh and Zaccolo groups reveals that a prototypical anchoring protein, known to be responsible for regulating several important physiological processes, also orchestrates the formation of two important universal second messengers.

Feeling tired? Here’s how the brain’s ‘hourglass’ controls your need for sleep – new research

New article on The Conversation website written by Dr Lukas Krone, Associate Professor Vladyslav Vyazovskiy and Professor Zoltán Molnár.

Scientists Decipher How NeuroImmune Interactions Burn Deep Fat

A pioneering collaborative mouse study from an international team of researchers including DPAG's Associate Professor Ana Domingos published in Nature offers new therapeutic avenues for reducing visceral fat stores, which have been associated with cardiovascular disease and multiple types of cancer.